This study sought to identify the psychosocial factors that influence handwashing behaviour and to design behaviour change techniques applying the risk, attitude, norm, ability, and self-regulation (RANAS) behaviour change approach in Rohingya camps in Cox's Bazar, Bangladesh. This was a cross-sectional study conducted in four Rohingya camps located in Ukhiya and Teknaf sub-districts. Participants were selected by systematic random sampling. A structured interviewer-administered questionnaire was used. Analysis of variance testing (ANOVA) was used to determine the significant predicting variables. A total of 1,358 individuals participated in the interview. The mean age was 31.11 ranging from 13 to 75 years. The majority were female (84.9%). Around 75% of participants reported practising handwashing. Several psychosocial predictors were identified for handwashing behaviour with medium to large effect size such as health knowledge, beliefs about costs and benefits, feelings, social norms, abilities, and self-regulation factors. Significant predictors were additionally identified in individual camps. The behaviour change techniques included providing information, demonstration of handwashing, providing rewards, evoking emotions, and public commitments amongst others proposed in this study. Identified psychosocial determinants should be considered while promoting sustainable handwashing behaviour along with adequate supplies and infrastructure. This study also suggests evaluating the proposed behaviour change interventions considering the contextual factors related to handwashing.

  • First structural assessment to identify psychosocial determinants of handwashing in Rohingya camps using the RANAS approach

  • Majority of the participants performed handwashing practices

  • Several factors predicted handwashing behaviours

  • Behaviour change interventions designed to address each psychosocial factor

Graphical Abstract

Graphical Abstract
Graphical Abstract

Hands are the vectors for transmitting diseases from human to humans; however, handwashing with soap and water at critical times has vast health implications (Mattioli et al. 2013; Ramos et al. 2016). Handwashing with soap at key times can limit morbidity and mortality from communicable diseases even in resource-limited settings (Rabie & Curtis 2006; Ejemot et al. 2008). In low- and middle-income regions, the prevalence of handwashing with soap is still extremely low with a mean ranging from 13% to 17% (Freeman et al. 2014). Diarrhoeal disease is one of the most common causes of death during humanitarian emergencies (Wisner & Adams 2012), which can be simply prevented by washing hands with soap, the most cost-effective hygiene promotion intervention (Cairncross et al. 2010). Handwashing, especially at critical times, is practised infrequently in stable refugee camps despite the strong link between handwashing and positive health outcomes (Biran et al. 2012).

Approximately 855,000 Rohingya refugees are currently residing in densely populated camps in Cox's Bazar, Bangladesh since the influx took place in August 2017. According to a recent report, 26% of households in the camps reported at least one individual having diarrhoea and 7% of all individuals had diarrhoea (REACH 2019). Children and women are at higher risk of diarrhoeal diseases in the camps (Islam et al. 2019). Evidence shows that the majority of people are aware of the importance of handwashing, but a minority regularly practises it through knowledge building interventions (Vivas et al. 2010). A recent assessment shows that, overall, 77% of respondents from a representative sample among the Rohingya population were able to name at least three critical handwashing times (Islam et al. 2019). Educational programmes help to promote handwashing but additional interventions addressing the determinants and drivers of handwashing are important to improve regular handwashing practices (Curtis et al. 2011). Applying behaviour change theories to promotion of interventions for handwashing may enhance their possibilities for behaviour change (Al-Tawfiq & Pittet 2013).

Promoting handwashing with soap has health benefits (Aiello et al. 2008; Talaat et al. 2011) that are well established, but the way to measure the behaviour change as an effect of promotional programmes is not confirmed (Ram 2013). Many studies have reported the health outcomes of handwashing interventions but lack the information on how the programme affected the handwashing behaviour (Cairncross et al. 2010). Moreover, many studies have revealed that hand hygiene practices are predicted by several psychological determinants of an individual (Scott et al. 2007; Lopez-Quintero et al. 2009; Curtis et al. 2011). In the Rohingya camps, we did not observe any structured assessment conducted to determine those psychological predictors for handwashing behaviour and any interventions that are based on evidence. In this study, we assessed the psychosocial factors that influenced handwashing behaviour using the RANAS (risks, attitudes, norms, abilities, and self-regulation) model of behaviour change (Mosler 2012). The RANAS approach is a systematic behaviour change approach particularly designed for behaviour change in the water, sanitation and hygiene (WASH) sector in developing countries. This model has been successfully applied to predict handwashing behaviour in many developing countries (Contzen & Mosler 2015; Seimetz et al. 2016, 2017) and the approach has also been proven effective to enhance handwashing practices in some regions of Ethiopia with water scarcity (Contzen et al. 2015). Therefore, our study aimed to identify the potential psychosocial factors that influenced handwashing behaviour which then contributed to the design of behaviour change interventions for the whole sample and specific to each camp.

Rohingya communities are settled in 34 different camps in two sub-districts (Ukhiya and Teknaf) of Cox's Bazar District in Bangladesh. Prior to the selection of those camps, we conducted a focus group discussion with WASH personnel from different organizations to identify the key areas for behaviour change assessments and interventions in the Rohingya camps. Then four organizations from Camp 2E, Camp 8E, Camp 8W and Camp 26 selected handwashing as a key area for behaviour change intervention.

The data we present here is the baseline data of a large pre-post behaviour change intervention project and therefore collected using cross-sectional design. We conducted face-to-face interviews with 1,358 households selected through systematic random sampling from multiple blocks of the selected camps. For interviews, we used a structured questionnaire based on the RANAS model (Mosler 2012). The RANAS model includes risk, attitude, norm, ability, and self-regulation factors, which is an effective tool to understand the association of behaviours with psychosocial aspects. The risk factors include one's perception of one's own vulnerability of getting a disease, severity of consequences after getting a disease, and knowledge about it (e.g. risk of getting diarrhoea if handwashing was not done properly at critical times). Attitude factors include feelings, and beliefs about costs and benefits of performing a specific behaviour (e.g. price of soap, time and difficulties of handwashing, benefits of handwashing). Norm factors entail the perception of a behaviour usually performed within a particular community, their approval or disapproval, and an individual's perceived obligation to perform it. Ability factors include the individual's capacity to carry out a behaviour (continue handwashing even when problems arise). The last component of the model, the self-regulation factor block, includes planning to perform a behaviour, commitment and remembering to perform the behaviour.

Including the sections on demographics and general habit of handwashing, several items in the questionnaires addressed the psychosocial factors according to the RANAS model (see questionnaires in the supplementary file). The interviewers were familiar with the Rohingya dialect having prior experience in conducting qualitative and quantitative interviews in the same context. We translated the main questionnaire into Bengali language and introduced it to the interviewers to test them in the field in the local Rohingya dialect. We measured the habit and other psychosocial factors in a 5-point Likert scale format with 1 as the minimum and 5 as the maximum for most of the questions. We have combined the score of two knowledge questions to calculate the overall health knowledge. We coded the correct responses as ‘1’ and incorrect responses as ‘0’ for health knowledge. We calculated the mean score for coping planning (self-regulation factor) using only two responses (yes or no, yes coded as ‘1’ and no coded as ‘0’).

We analysed the data using IBM SPSS Statistics software package version 26.0 (IBM 2019). To present the socio-demographic characteristics of study participants, we used descriptive statistics. We performed variance testing (ANOVA) to conduct a doers and non-doer's analysis in order to determine the significant predictor variable. We also calculated the effect size for each factor for the whole sample and individually for each camp using Cohen's d equation and determined the small, medium and large effect size (Cohen 2013).

A total of 1,358 Rohingya participants attended the interviews from four different camps. The mean age was 31.11 ± 10.17 (SD). The majority of the participants were 30 years of age or below (n= 825, 60.8%). A vast majority of participants were female (n = 1,153, 84.9%). The average household size for the whole sample was 5.50 ± 2.38 (SD). The average household size of the individual camps was 5.64 ± 3.18 (SD) in Camp 8W, 5.97 ± 2.13 (SD) in Camp 8E, 5.54 ± 2.13 (SD) in Camp 26, and 5.02 ± 2.05 (SD) in Camp 2E. Among the surveyed population, the majority were illiterate (n = 997, 73.4%) followed by primary education (n = 116, 8.5%). The average monthly family income was 2,368.04 Bangladeshi Taka (BDT) (SD = 2,805.427) ranging from 0 to 21,000 BDT (USD 1 ≈ BDT 85.10). Nearly 50% (n = 657, 48.4%) of people in this surveyed population had an income of less than BDT 5,000 and 32.4% (n= 440) of people had no income (Table 1).

Table 1

Socio-demographic characteristics of participants

CharacteristicsWhole sample (N= 1,358)
Camp 8W (N= 273)
Camp 8E (N= 291)
Camp 26 (N= 394)
Camp 2E (N= 400)
n%n%n%n%n%
Age (years) 
≤ 30 825 60.8 119 43.6 185 63.6 292 74.1 229 57.3 
31–50 465 34.2 124 45.4 91 31.3 94 23.9 156 39.0 
> 50 68 5.0 30 11.0 15 5.2 2.0 15 3.8 
Gender 
Male 205 15.1 123 45.1 39 13.4 28 7.1 15 3.8 
Female 1,153 84.9 150 54.9 252 86.6 366 92.9 385 96.3 
Educational level 
None or do not know 997 73.4 184 67.4 188 64.6 244 61.9 381 95.3 
Can read but not write 110 8.1 0.7 20 6.9 86 21.8 0.5 
Can read and write 59 4.3 14 5.1 16 5.5 17 4.3 12 3.0 
Primary 116 8.5 41 15.0 56 19.2 15 3.8 1.0 
Secondary 39 2.9 27 9.9 11 3.8 0.3 
College and higher 0.2 1.1 
Other 34 2.5 0.7 32 8.1 
Income 
No income 440 32.4 124 45.4 26 8.9 145 36.8 145 36.3 
<BDT 5,000 657 48.4 89 32.6 195 67.0 152 38.6 221 55.3 
BDT 5,000–15,000 256 18.9 57 20.9 69 23.7 97 24.6 33 8.3 
BDT > 15,000 0.4 1.1 0.3 0.3 
CharacteristicsWhole sample (N= 1,358)
Camp 8W (N= 273)
Camp 8E (N= 291)
Camp 26 (N= 394)
Camp 2E (N= 400)
n%n%n%n%n%
Age (years) 
≤ 30 825 60.8 119 43.6 185 63.6 292 74.1 229 57.3 
31–50 465 34.2 124 45.4 91 31.3 94 23.9 156 39.0 
> 50 68 5.0 30 11.0 15 5.2 2.0 15 3.8 
Gender 
Male 205 15.1 123 45.1 39 13.4 28 7.1 15 3.8 
Female 1,153 84.9 150 54.9 252 86.6 366 92.9 385 96.3 
Educational level 
None or do not know 997 73.4 184 67.4 188 64.6 244 61.9 381 95.3 
Can read but not write 110 8.1 0.7 20 6.9 86 21.8 0.5 
Can read and write 59 4.3 14 5.1 16 5.5 17 4.3 12 3.0 
Primary 116 8.5 41 15.0 56 19.2 15 3.8 1.0 
Secondary 39 2.9 27 9.9 11 3.8 0.3 
College and higher 0.2 1.1 
Other 34 2.5 0.7 32 8.1 
Income 
No income 440 32.4 124 45.4 26 8.9 145 36.8 145 36.3 
<BDT 5,000 657 48.4 89 32.6 195 67.0 152 38.6 221 55.3 
BDT 5,000–15,000 256 18.9 57 20.9 69 23.7 97 24.6 33 8.3 
BDT > 15,000 0.4 1.1 0.3 0.3 

Several items in the questionnaire addressed the different aspects of handwashing behaviour among the participants. Bucket (n = 869, 64.0%) and can (n= 864, 63.6%) were the devices mostly used to wet hands among the whole surveyed population. Nearly all of the participants wash both their hands (n = 1,328, 97.8%). Around 98% (n = 1,321) of participants used soap for handwashing. The mean self-reported frequency of handwashing in general for the whole sample was 5.65 times. For individual camps, the mean frequency was 6.4 times in Camp 8W, 5.4 times in Camp 8E, 5.4 times in Camp 26, and 5.6 times in Camp 2E. Regarding the self-reported habit of handwashing, the majority of the participants replied on a scale from 1 (not at all habitually) to 5 (very habitually) that they ‘habitually’ washed their hands (n = 892, 65.7%) (Table 2). To determine the doers and non-doers of handwashing, we have used the question assessing habitual handwashing and decided for a cut-off on the 5-point Likert scale. Participants who answered ‘1–3’ on the Likert scale were considered as ‘non-doers’, and those who answered ‘4–5’ on the Likert scale were considered as ‘doers’. By applying this cut-off 75% were classified as doers (n = 1,023, 75.3%). We found the highest number of ‘doers’ in Camp 2E (83.8%) (Table 2).

Table 2

Handwashing behaviour of participants

CharacteristicsWhole sample (N= 1,358)
Camp 8W (N= 273)
Camp 8E (N= 291)
Camp 26 (N= 394)
Camp 2E (N= 400)
n%n%n%n%n%
Device used to wet hands 
Bucket 869 64.0 188 30.8 216 74.2 179 45.4 286 71.5 
Can 864 63.6 180 68.9 208 71.5 249 63.2 227 56.8 
Plastic bottle 26 1.9 0.7 18 6.2 0.5 1.0 
Sink filled 48 3.5 1.1 0.7 43 10.9 
Flowing water from tap 151 11.1 31 11.4 2.7 112 28.0 
Hand(s) washed 
Both hands 1,328 97.8 272 99.6 284 97.6 393 99.7 379 94.8 
Left hand only 14 1.0 0.4 1.0 10 2.5 
Right hand only 18 1.3 1.4 0.3 13 3.3 
Detergent used for handwashing 
Soap 1,321 97.3 273 100 262 90.0 391 99.2 395 98.8 
Ash 10 0.7 0.7 0.3 1.8 
Soapy water 15 1.1 2.9 0.7 0.3 1.0 
Mud 0.1 0.7 
None 27 2.0 27 9.3 
Habit of handwashinga 
Not habitually at all 16 1.2 13 4.5 0.8 
A little bit habitually 230 16.9 62 22.7 42 14.4 77 19.5 49 12.3 
Rather habitually 89 6.6 33 12.1 32 11.0 11 2.8 13 3.3 
Habitually 892 65.7 145 53.1 161 55.3 306 77.7 280 70.0 
Very habitually 131 9.6 33 12.1 43 14.8 55 13.8 
Doers/Non-doers of handwashinga 
Non-doers 335 24.7 95 34.8 87 29.9 88 22.3 65 16.3 
Doers 1,023 75.3 178 65.2 204 70.1 306 77.7 335 83.8 
CharacteristicsWhole sample (N= 1,358)
Camp 8W (N= 273)
Camp 8E (N= 291)
Camp 26 (N= 394)
Camp 2E (N= 400)
n%n%n%n%n%
Device used to wet hands 
Bucket 869 64.0 188 30.8 216 74.2 179 45.4 286 71.5 
Can 864 63.6 180 68.9 208 71.5 249 63.2 227 56.8 
Plastic bottle 26 1.9 0.7 18 6.2 0.5 1.0 
Sink filled 48 3.5 1.1 0.7 43 10.9 
Flowing water from tap 151 11.1 31 11.4 2.7 112 28.0 
Hand(s) washed 
Both hands 1,328 97.8 272 99.6 284 97.6 393 99.7 379 94.8 
Left hand only 14 1.0 0.4 1.0 10 2.5 
Right hand only 18 1.3 1.4 0.3 13 3.3 
Detergent used for handwashing 
Soap 1,321 97.3 273 100 262 90.0 391 99.2 395 98.8 
Ash 10 0.7 0.7 0.3 1.8 
Soapy water 15 1.1 2.9 0.7 0.3 1.0 
Mud 0.1 0.7 
None 27 2.0 27 9.3 
Habit of handwashinga 
Not habitually at all 16 1.2 13 4.5 0.8 
A little bit habitually 230 16.9 62 22.7 42 14.4 77 19.5 49 12.3 
Rather habitually 89 6.6 33 12.1 32 11.0 11 2.8 13 3.3 
Habitually 892 65.7 145 53.1 161 55.3 306 77.7 280 70.0 
Very habitually 131 9.6 33 12.1 43 14.8 55 13.8 
Doers/Non-doers of handwashinga 
Non-doers 335 24.7 95 34.8 87 29.9 88 22.3 65 16.3 
Doers 1,023 75.3 178 65.2 204 70.1 306 77.7 335 83.8 

aHow habitually do you wash your hands with soap and water before handling food?.

In a next step, we compared the doers and non-doers and calculated the mean difference for the two groups on each RANAS factor for the whole sample and individually for each camp. Then we calculated the effect size of each factor. We focused on those RANAS factors with significant mean difference and medium to large effect size (Table 3). Those are the relevant factors that need to be targeted in order to design effective behaviour change interventions. We display first the results for the whole sample and then specific findings of individual camps. Participants who washed their hands less habitually had lower levels of health knowledge (Risk factors: health knowledge) and reported less to like handwashing with soap and water before handling food (Attitude factors: liking washing hands). They felt less disgusted when not washing hands with soap and water before handling food (Attitude factors: disgust). Participants who washed their hands less habitually, think that many people in their community wash their hands at a low frequency (Norm factors: others’ behaviour, community members). They also think that many of their relatives wash their hands at a low frequency with soap and water before handling food (Norm factors: others’ behaviour, relatives). Participants with less habitual handwashing behaviour also think that washing hands with soap and water does not make them a respected person in the community (Norm factors: personal respect) and did not see themselves as role models in the community (Norm factors: role model). Participants in the whole sample who washed their hands less habitually were also less confident about the fact that they can always wash their hands with soap and water before handling food (Ability factors: confidence performance). They were also less confident about maintaining handwashing behaviour with soap and water before handling food, especially when they are busy (Ability factors: confidence maintenance). Participants who washed their hands less habitually had fewer plans about when during the day they would wash their hands with soap and water (Self-regulation factors: action plan) and were also less committed to performing the behaviour (Self-regulation factors: commitment). In some individual camps, some factors seem to be of special importance for handwashing as they have a large effect size. For example, health knowledge (Risk factor) in Camp 2E, personal respect (Norm factor) in Camp 8W, action control (Self-regulation factor) in Camp 26, and action plan, coping plan, and commitment (Self-regulation factors) in Camp 2E.

Table 3

Mean difference of doers and non-doers and effect size of RANAS factors

Whole sample
Camp 8W
Camp 8E
Camp 26
Camp 2E
FactorsMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect size
Risk     
Vulnerability 1 0.11 0.1 0.79*** 0.57 0.07 0.06 0.05 0.08 − 0.08 0.09 
Vulnerability 2 0.12 0.15 0.21* 0.28 0.25* 0.38 0.07 0.11 − 0.06 0.08 
Severity 0.86 0.12 0.11 0.25 0.19* 0.27 − 0.12 0.21 0.36** 0.58 
Health knowledge 1.01*** 0.46 − 0.40 0.24 1.30*** 0.59 0.36 0.21 2.22*** 1.49 
Attitude 
Beliefs about costs and benefits (germ free) 0.13* 0.16 0.20 0.21 0.23** 0.36 − 0.24*** 0.34 0.58*** 0.61 
Beliefs about costs and benefits (time-consuming) 0.10 0.10 0.62*** 0.51 − 0.38* 0.36 0.39*** 0.49 − 0.20* 0.29 
Feelings 
1. Like/Dislike washing hands 0.21*** 0.42 0.28*** 0.59 0.26*** 0.51 0.02 0.09 0.32*** 0.53 
2. Smell of soap 0.13*** 0.22 − 0.05 0.09 0.19* 0.31 0.19*** 0.46 0.20* 0.30 
3. Disgust − 0.42*** 0.35 − 0.83*** 0.66 − 0.39** 0.37 0.19* 0.18 − 0.54** 0.42 
4 Clean/ Dirty 0.20*** 0.19 0.23 0.26 0.28* 0.28 − 0.05 0.11 0.44** 0.53 
5. Comfortable 0.12** 0.15 0.78*** 0.61 − 0.03 0.03 0.01 0.06 0.04 0.08 
Norms 
Others’ behaviour (Number of people in the community) 0.37*** 0.38 − 0.02 0.0 0.28* 0.30 0.39*** 0.73 0.31** 0.39 
Others’ behaviour (Number of relatives) 0.40*** 0.41 0.11 0.19 0.44** 0.46 0.41*** 0.69 0.28* 0.35 
Others’ (dis)approval (People who are important) 0.13*** 0.18 − 0.03 0.03 0.23* 0.29 0.01 0.06 0.16* 0.28 
Personal importance 
1. Important/ Unimportant 0.23*** 0.36 0.24** 0.38 0.38*** 0.72 − 0.01 0.06 0.44** 0.50 
2. Respected person 0.26*** 0.34 0.54*** 0.87 0.46*** 0.58 − 0.21*** 0.49 0.38** 0.41 
3. Role-model 0.26*** 0.33 0.58*** 0.73 0.40** 0.41 − 0.16** 0.25 0.30* 0.36 
Abilities 
Confidence in performance 0.19*** 0.41 0.31*** 0.70 0.40*** 0.71 − 0.06 0.24 0.22** 0.48 
Confidence in continuation 0.27*** 0.51 0.32*** 0.78 0.48*** 0.72 − 0.02 0.12 0.28*** 0.49 
Self-regulation 
Action control 0.05 0.05 0.31** 0.30 0.70*** 0.77 − 0.63*** 0.88 − 0.18 0.14 
Action plan 0.80*** 0.50 0.03 0.02 0.46** 0.38 0.38* 0.25 2.13*** 1.45 
Coping plan 0.08** 0.24 0.13** 0.31 0.04 0.10 − 0.01 0.06 0.39*** 1.34 
Remembering 0.04 0.05 0.08 0.08 0.51*** 0.55 − 0.87*** 0.79 0.30** 0.36 
Lazy − 0.09 0.07 0.40* 0.27 − 0.77*** 0.55 1.01*** 0.79 − 0.31 0.24 
Commitment 0.39*** 0.57 0.37*** 0.58 0.44*** 0.45 0.02 0.08 0.71*** 0.99 
Whole sample
Camp 8W
Camp 8E
Camp 26
Camp 2E
FactorsMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect sizeMean differenceEffect size
Risk     
Vulnerability 1 0.11 0.1 0.79*** 0.57 0.07 0.06 0.05 0.08 − 0.08 0.09 
Vulnerability 2 0.12 0.15 0.21* 0.28 0.25* 0.38 0.07 0.11 − 0.06 0.08 
Severity 0.86 0.12 0.11 0.25 0.19* 0.27 − 0.12 0.21 0.36** 0.58 
Health knowledge 1.01*** 0.46 − 0.40 0.24 1.30*** 0.59 0.36 0.21 2.22*** 1.49 
Attitude 
Beliefs about costs and benefits (germ free) 0.13* 0.16 0.20 0.21 0.23** 0.36 − 0.24*** 0.34 0.58*** 0.61 
Beliefs about costs and benefits (time-consuming) 0.10 0.10 0.62*** 0.51 − 0.38* 0.36 0.39*** 0.49 − 0.20* 0.29 
Feelings 
1. Like/Dislike washing hands 0.21*** 0.42 0.28*** 0.59 0.26*** 0.51 0.02 0.09 0.32*** 0.53 
2. Smell of soap 0.13*** 0.22 − 0.05 0.09 0.19* 0.31 0.19*** 0.46 0.20* 0.30 
3. Disgust − 0.42*** 0.35 − 0.83*** 0.66 − 0.39** 0.37 0.19* 0.18 − 0.54** 0.42 
4 Clean/ Dirty 0.20*** 0.19 0.23 0.26 0.28* 0.28 − 0.05 0.11 0.44** 0.53 
5. Comfortable 0.12** 0.15 0.78*** 0.61 − 0.03 0.03 0.01 0.06 0.04 0.08 
Norms 
Others’ behaviour (Number of people in the community) 0.37*** 0.38 − 0.02 0.0 0.28* 0.30 0.39*** 0.73 0.31** 0.39 
Others’ behaviour (Number of relatives) 0.40*** 0.41 0.11 0.19 0.44** 0.46 0.41*** 0.69 0.28* 0.35 
Others’ (dis)approval (People who are important) 0.13*** 0.18 − 0.03 0.03 0.23* 0.29 0.01 0.06 0.16* 0.28 
Personal importance 
1. Important/ Unimportant 0.23*** 0.36 0.24** 0.38 0.38*** 0.72 − 0.01 0.06 0.44** 0.50 
2. Respected person 0.26*** 0.34 0.54*** 0.87 0.46*** 0.58 − 0.21*** 0.49 0.38** 0.41 
3. Role-model 0.26*** 0.33 0.58*** 0.73 0.40** 0.41 − 0.16** 0.25 0.30* 0.36 
Abilities 
Confidence in performance 0.19*** 0.41 0.31*** 0.70 0.40*** 0.71 − 0.06 0.24 0.22** 0.48 
Confidence in continuation 0.27*** 0.51 0.32*** 0.78 0.48*** 0.72 − 0.02 0.12 0.28*** 0.49 
Self-regulation 
Action control 0.05 0.05 0.31** 0.30 0.70*** 0.77 − 0.63*** 0.88 − 0.18 0.14 
Action plan 0.80*** 0.50 0.03 0.02 0.46** 0.38 0.38* 0.25 2.13*** 1.45 
Coping plan 0.08** 0.24 0.13** 0.31 0.04 0.10 − 0.01 0.06 0.39*** 1.34 
Remembering 0.04 0.05 0.08 0.08 0.51*** 0.55 − 0.87*** 0.79 0.30** 0.36 
Lazy − 0.09 0.07 0.40* 0.27 − 0.77*** 0.55 1.01*** 0.79 − 0.31 0.24 
Commitment 0.39*** 0.57 0.37*** 0.58 0.44*** 0.45 0.02 0.08 0.71*** 0.99 

*p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

The following findings might imply interventions on additional behavioural factors in individual camps but did not show up in the whole sample. In Camp 8W, participants with less habitual handwashing behaviour thought that there is no risk of becoming sick if they do not wash their hands with soap and water before handling food (Risk factors: vulnerability (1). Similarly, in Camp 8E, participants with less habitual handwashing behaviour do not think that the risk of becoming sick is high if they do not wash their hands properly with soap and water (Risk factors: vulnerability (2). People with less habitual handwashing behaviour in Camp 2E also think that the impact on their life of becoming sick from not washing hands properly is not so severe (Risk factors: severity). The population in Camp 8E and Camp 2E who washed their hands less habitually think that washing hands with soap and water does not make them germ free (Attitude factors: germ free). For the population in Camp 8E and Camp 26 with less habitual handwashing behaviour, washing hands with soap and water was quite time-consuming (Attitude factors: time-consuming). People with less habitual handwashing behaviour in Camp 8E, Camp 26, and Camp 2E did not like the smell of soap on their hands after washing (Attitude factors: smell of soap). In Camp 2E, participants who washed their hands less habitually did not feel clean after washing hands with soap and water before handling food (Attitude factors: feeling clean). In Camp 8W, participants with less habitual handwashing behaviour did not feel comfortable after washing hands with soap and water (Attitude factors: feeling comfortable). People in Camp 8W and 8E who washed their hands less habitually had washed their hands more only with water but without soap (Self-regulation factors: action control). People in Camp 8W and Camp 26 with less habitual handwashing behaviour did not have a plan to cope with barriers for handwashing (Self-regulation factors: coping plan). People who washed their hands less habitually also forget to wash their hands with soap and water (Self-regulation factors: remembering). This was true for people in Camp 8E and Camp 2E. People in Camp 8E with less habitual handwashing behaviour agreed that they are sometimes too lazy to wash their hands with soap and water (Self-regulation factors: lazy) (Table 3).

In this study, we used the RANAS model to identify the potential psychosocial factors of handwashing behaviour among the Rohingya refugees in Bangladesh. Then we designed behaviour change interventions accordingly using the RANAS approach (Table 4). Behaviour change techniques (BCTs) can influence or redirect the process that controls a behaviour, and should therefore be included as part of intervention planning. The designed BCTs against each psychosocial factor were derived from the catalogue of BCTs provided in the RANAS guideline (Mosler & Contzen 2016).

Table 4

Behaviour change interventions

RANAS blockRANAS factorsInterventionsCommunication channels
Risk Vulnerability Demonstration of risk if anyone doesn't wash their hands properly (BCT 3)
Specific for Camp 8W and Camp 8E 
Community meeting with video or storytelling 
Severity Provide threating information and severity of contracting a disease if they do not wash hands properly (BCT 4)
Specific for Camp 2E 
Health knowledgea Short information about washing hands properly at key times (BCT 1 & 2) Mosque-based discussions 
Attitude Beliefs about costs and benefits (germ-free) Handwashing demonstration to show how hands get germ free, provide information about costs and benefits of handwashing (BCT 5) and reward a person each time he or she performs handwashing and makes it germ-free (BCT 6).
Specific for Camp 8E, Camp 26, and Camp 2E 
Community meeting with video or storytelling 
Beliefs about costs and benefits (time-consuming) Invite participants to talk to others about how handwashing saves time and health (BCT 7).
Specific for Camp 8E and Camp 26 
Feelingsa Find people who already wash their hands at key times. Those role models shall talk about their positive experiences with washing hands properly at key times, why and how they do it and why it is good for everyone to do it. Evoke emotions. Message: I feel clean and comfortable when washing hands at key times with soap and water, I feel disgusted when not washing hands properly (BCT 8). Community meeting, mosque-based discussions 
Norm Others’ behavioura Find parents or other family members who already wash hands properly at key times. Emphasize women as they do not have the same access as men to a mosque (BCT 9). Make people commit to handwashing in public (BCT 10). Community meeting, mosque-based discussions 
Others’ (dis)approvala Important leaders and authority figures like the Imam and the Majhi talk about washing hands properly at key times in a positive way and express that they approve of it. They tell parents how important it is to help their children and elderly to wash hands properly at key times (BCT 11). Community meeting, mosque-based discussions 
Ability Confidence in performancea Spot check: observation and joint monitoring at the handwashing site and support the community to set up infrastructure (BCT 16). Demonstrate handwashing and prompt participants to pay attention to others’ performing the behaviour and its consequences in their everyday life (BCT 17). Train participants on handwashing through guided practice and giving feedback (BCT 18 & 19). Household visits, community meeting with video or storytelling 
 Confidence in continuationa Prompt participants that failures to wash hands at key times are a temporary lack of skill or adverse circumstances instead of personal deficiency and their successes are personal achievements. Household visits 
Self-regulation Action control Prompt participants to monitor their own behaviour by means of recording it (BCT 27) and provide feedback on performance (BCT 28).
Specific to Camp 8W, Camp 8E, and Camp 26 
Household visits 
Action plana Prompt participants not only to do handwashing, but also when, where and how they intend to achieve their goals (BCT 26) 
Coping plan Invite role models who already wash hands properly at key times to talk about their positive experiences and give recommendations about problem solving then ask participants to identify their barriers and plan for solutions (BCT 30).
Specific for Camp 8W and Camp 2E 
Mosque-based discussion, community meeting 
Remembering Prompt to remember handwashing at key times. Show compassion for how difficult it is to remember each key time when washing hands properly is really important. Introduce a bracelet with handwashing images. Explain each image as a key time to wash hands properly (BCT 34).
Specific for Camp 8E, Camp 26, and Camp 2E 
Household visits 
Lazy Prompt participants to exploit environmental cues so as to trigger handwashing in the right situation (BCT 34).
Specific for Camp 8E and Camp 26 
Commitmenta Behavioural contract to wash hands properly at key times (BCT 36). 
RANAS blockRANAS factorsInterventionsCommunication channels
Risk Vulnerability Demonstration of risk if anyone doesn't wash their hands properly (BCT 3)
Specific for Camp 8W and Camp 8E 
Community meeting with video or storytelling 
Severity Provide threating information and severity of contracting a disease if they do not wash hands properly (BCT 4)
Specific for Camp 2E 
Health knowledgea Short information about washing hands properly at key times (BCT 1 & 2) Mosque-based discussions 
Attitude Beliefs about costs and benefits (germ-free) Handwashing demonstration to show how hands get germ free, provide information about costs and benefits of handwashing (BCT 5) and reward a person each time he or she performs handwashing and makes it germ-free (BCT 6).
Specific for Camp 8E, Camp 26, and Camp 2E 
Community meeting with video or storytelling 
Beliefs about costs and benefits (time-consuming) Invite participants to talk to others about how handwashing saves time and health (BCT 7).
Specific for Camp 8E and Camp 26 
Feelingsa Find people who already wash their hands at key times. Those role models shall talk about their positive experiences with washing hands properly at key times, why and how they do it and why it is good for everyone to do it. Evoke emotions. Message: I feel clean and comfortable when washing hands at key times with soap and water, I feel disgusted when not washing hands properly (BCT 8). Community meeting, mosque-based discussions 
Norm Others’ behavioura Find parents or other family members who already wash hands properly at key times. Emphasize women as they do not have the same access as men to a mosque (BCT 9). Make people commit to handwashing in public (BCT 10). Community meeting, mosque-based discussions 
Others’ (dis)approvala Important leaders and authority figures like the Imam and the Majhi talk about washing hands properly at key times in a positive way and express that they approve of it. They tell parents how important it is to help their children and elderly to wash hands properly at key times (BCT 11). Community meeting, mosque-based discussions 
Ability Confidence in performancea Spot check: observation and joint monitoring at the handwashing site and support the community to set up infrastructure (BCT 16). Demonstrate handwashing and prompt participants to pay attention to others’ performing the behaviour and its consequences in their everyday life (BCT 17). Train participants on handwashing through guided practice and giving feedback (BCT 18 & 19). Household visits, community meeting with video or storytelling 
 Confidence in continuationa Prompt participants that failures to wash hands at key times are a temporary lack of skill or adverse circumstances instead of personal deficiency and their successes are personal achievements. Household visits 
Self-regulation Action control Prompt participants to monitor their own behaviour by means of recording it (BCT 27) and provide feedback on performance (BCT 28).
Specific to Camp 8W, Camp 8E, and Camp 26 
Household visits 
Action plana Prompt participants not only to do handwashing, but also when, where and how they intend to achieve their goals (BCT 26) 
Coping plan Invite role models who already wash hands properly at key times to talk about their positive experiences and give recommendations about problem solving then ask participants to identify their barriers and plan for solutions (BCT 30).
Specific for Camp 8W and Camp 2E 
Mosque-based discussion, community meeting 
Remembering Prompt to remember handwashing at key times. Show compassion for how difficult it is to remember each key time when washing hands properly is really important. Introduce a bracelet with handwashing images. Explain each image as a key time to wash hands properly (BCT 34).
Specific for Camp 8E, Camp 26, and Camp 2E 
Household visits 
Lazy Prompt participants to exploit environmental cues so as to trigger handwashing in the right situation (BCT 34).
Specific for Camp 8E and Camp 26 
Commitmenta Behavioural contract to wash hands properly at key times (BCT 36). 

BCT, behaviour change technique.

aInterventions designed for the whole sample.

For the whole sample, the behaviour change interventions were designed to address health knowledge (risk factor), liking of handwashing and disgust (attitude factors), others’ behaviour and personal importance (norm factors), confidence in performance and continuation (ability factors), and action planning and commitment (self-regulation factors). RANAS factors such as vulnerability, severity, beliefs about cost and benefit, feelings (smell of soap, feeling lazy, and comfortable), action control, remembering, or coping plan, were considered individually for different camps (Table 4). This will help the policy makers (camp authority, implementing partners, etc.) in the camp to design their hygiene promotion programmes in order to improve handwashing practices.

The results show that the self-reported habit of handwashing (doers) among the study participants was high in the whole sample as well as in individual camps. The results in this study also show that participants with low self-reported handwashing habit had low levels of perceived health risk (knowledge of diarrhoea) (Risk factor: health knowledge). Evidence also suggests that increasing awareness of handwashing importance and increasing hygiene knowledge improves proper handwashing practices (O'Reilly et al. 2008; Patel et al. 2012; Saboori et al. 2013). Therefore, WASH implementing agencies in the surveyed camps can design hygiene promotion activities considering the following psychosocial factors identified in this study and behaviour change techniques to address each individual factor.

Two attitude factors, liking handwashing and feeling of disgust, were significant predictors of handwashing for doers. Participants who felt less disgusted at not washing hands with soap and water washed their hands less habitually. A study conducted in Haiti and Ethiopia had similar findings where people tended to wash their hands more frequently than others when they believed that it is disgusting not to wash their hands after defecation or before preparing food (Contzen et al. 2015). The importance of disgust in triggering handwashing is also reported in many other studies (Porzig-Drummond et al. 2009; Aunger et al. 2010). To address those factors, WASH agencies can find role models who wash their hands at key times and ask them to share their positive feelings and experiences, and share why and how they washed their hands and why it is important for everyone. The idea is to evoke emotions among the non-doers of handwashing. These interventions can be delivered through community group meetings and mosque-based discussion at camps (Table 4).

Multiple norm factors were of significant importance to predict handwashing behaviour. People who washed their hands less habitually also believed that a significant number of people in their community and relatives were also less likely to wash their hands. Similar conclusions were drawn in many other different types of studies (Judah et al. 2009; Aunger et al. 2010; World Bank 2012; Contzen et al. 2015). Participants with high handwashing habits also believed that washing hands with soap and water makes a person respected and a role model in their community. The potential behaviour change interventions to address the norm factors are to find family members or relatives who wash their hands at key times and ask them to share their experiences in front of the community. People should also make commitments in public that they would wash their hands regularly. Community leaders (Imam and Majhi) and other authority figures should also share their positive experience about handwashing and express that they approve of handwashing by the community members (Table 4). Implementing partners can use those techniques through community meetings and mosque-based discussion as a channel of communication.

Two ability factors had significant mean difference between the doers and non-doers. Participants who washed their hands more habitually were also confident to wash their hands at all times. The result was in line with the findings of a study conducted in Haiti and Ethiopia (Contzen et al. 2015). Confidence in maintaining handwashing behaviour even during busy days or with impediments was high among the doers in this study. It is also evident that certain impediments or access influence handwashing habits (World Bank 2012). Spot check observations of handwashing infrastructure, handwashing demonstration publicly, guided handwashing practice and giving feedback through hygiene promoters are some of the interventions to address those ability factors. The WASH actors in the camps can deliver those interventions by household visits, community meetings, video or storytelling (Table 4).

Two self-regulation factors, action planning and commitment, were of significant importance to predict handwashing habits among doers and non-doers for the whole sample. Participants who had more plans for handwashing tended to wash hands more habitually. Action planning was also a predictor of effective handwashing techniques in a study in Zimbabwe (Friedrich et al. 2017). Participants who had high handwashing habits were also more committed to handwashing with soap and water. This finding was in accordance with another relevant study conducted in Haiti and Ethiopia (Contzen et al. 2015). In order to help participants with planning and committing towards handwashing, WASH agencies prompt community members not only to wash hands but also to plan when, where and how they intend to achieve this goal. Hygiene promoters can also ask for a behavioural contract of handwashing from the community members. The above-mentioned interventions can be delivered through household visits (Table 4).

Looking at the individual camps, we have identified other psychosocial factors of significant importance among the doers and non-doers. For example, lower perceived vulnerability to diarrhoea was found among the non-doers in Camp 8W and Camp 8E which is also a significant determinant for handwashing technique in a study in Zimbabwe (Friedrich et al. 2017). To address the vulnerability in both of the camps, hygiene promoters can demonstrate the risk if anyone does not wash their hands properly. The channel of communication could be community meeting, video or storytelling (Table 4). In Camp 8W and Camp 2E, participants who had plans to deal with obstructions washed their hands more habitually (Self-regulation factor: coping planning). Similarity was also observed in another study (Contzen et al. 2015). To address coping planning among the community members, hygiene promoters can invite role models in the community who already wash their hands at key times to talk about their positive experiences and give recommendations about problem solving for handwashing. Then they can also ask the community members to talk about their barriers and help them to plan and find solutions. This intervention can be delivered through mosque-based discussion or community meetings in those camps. Participants with handwashing habits in Camp 8E, Camp 26, and Camp 2E stated that they remember to wash hands even when they are in a rush. Similar findings were also observed in a study in Haiti and Ethiopia (Contzen et al. 2015); though the finding is opposite in Camp 26 (negative mean difference), where participants with less handwashing habits tend to remember to wash hands even when they are in a hurry. In many cases, we have observed different findings in Camp 26, which is difficult to explain (negative mean difference between doers and non-doers) (Table 3). A possible explanation could be that the Rohingya population in Camp 26 live nearby the registered camp, which was established approximately 30 years ago, and have embraced the habits of people from that camp, or they may have arrived in Bangladesh long ago (approximately 30 years), therefore they have adapted their behaviour in a different way or are less attentive to the promotional activities, unlike the new arrivals.

It is therefore recommended to the WASH implementing agencies working in those Rohingya camps to tailor their hygiene promotion activities for handwashing following the above identified psychosocial factors and proposed interventions targeting the non-doers of handwashing. The listed communication channel would help to deliver the interventions which were finalized following a discussion with the hygiene promoters working in the camps. WASH agencies working in other camps can also use these findings and interventions, and tailor their activities, as all camp populations live in the same context and mostly share similar socio-economic conditions, cultural values, and similar levels of service from the national and international organizations providing services in the camps. The WASH sector can also use the findings as evidence and recommend the designed interventions as a sector-wide approach for handwashing in a hygiene promotion strategy.

There was no similar study found in this context; therefore, we could not compare our results. We designed this survey in a cross-sectional manner, meaning the results do not signify causality. Most of the results shown in this study were self-reported behaviours of participants and therefore should be considered with caution. Evidence shows that self-reported handwashing behaviour overestimates when compared to the actual observations (Curtis et al. 1993; Manun'Ebo et al. 1997). The use of a long questionnaire for the interview was another limitation. However, field practice made the interviewers familiar with all questions and they were able to save time. However, selecting interviewers from the local host community who are familiar with the local dialect, training, and pre-testing of the questionnaire encountered this issue.

We observed high frequency of self-reported handwashing habit among the surveyed participants. Health knowledge, liking handwashing, feeling disgusted, beliefs about cost and benefits, personal importance, confidence in performance and continuation, action plan, and commitment are the potential psychosocial factors determining handwashing behaviour for the whole sample. These factors should be considered in handwashing promotional events. The proposed behaviour change interventions in this study will guide the service providers to implement handwashing programmes using different communication channels to improve sustainable handwashing practices along with adequate infrastructure and supplies.

The authors are grateful to the study participants for their contribution to the research, as well as the partner organizations and the camp authority in the particular camps where the study was carried out. We are also grateful to the WASH team of UNICEF and WASH sector coordination unit, DPHE, Cox's Bazar for their cooperation in each stage of this study.

This study was a part of the UNICEF hygiene promotion intervention project entitled ‘Integration of Risks, Attitudes, Norms, Abilities, and Self-Regulation (RANAS) methodology for behaviour change in Hygiene Promotion approaches in the WASH Rohingya response: Analysis, testing and evaluation’. The project obtained ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board of the Institute of Health Economics (IHE-IRB), University of Dhaka, which is approved by Federalwide Assurance (FWA), No. FWA00026031.

United Nations Children's Fund (UNICEF) funded this study.

Data cannot be made publicly available; readers should contact the corresponding author for details.

The authors declare there is no conflict.

Aiello
A. E.
,
Coulborn
R. M.
,
Perez
V.
&
Larson
E. L.
2008
Effect of hand hygiene on infectious disease risk in the community setting: a meta-analysis
.
Am. J. Public Health
98
(
8
),
1372
1381
.
https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2007.124610
.
Aunger
R.
,
Schmidt
W. P.
,
Ranpura
A.
,
Coombes
Y.
,
Maina
P. M.
,
Matiko
C. N.
&
Curtis
V.
2010
Three kinds of psychological determinants for hand-washing behaviour in Kenya
.
Soc. Sci. Med.
70
(
3
),
383
391
.
Biran
A.
,
Schmidt
W. P.
,
Zeleke
L.
,
Emukule
H.
,
Khay
H.
,
Parker
J.
&
Peprah
D.
2012
Hygiene and sanitation practices amongst residents of three long-term refugee camps in Thailand, Ethiopia and Kenya
.
Trop. Med. Int. Health
17
(
9
),
1133
1141
.
Cairncross
S.
,
Hunt
C.
,
Boisson
S.
,
Bostoen
K.
,
Curtis
V.
,
Fung
I. C. H.
&
Schmidt
W. P.
2010
Water, sanitation and hygiene for the prevention of diarrhoea
.
Int. J. Epidemiol.
39
(
Suppl. 1
),
i193
i205
.
Cohen
J.
2013
Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences
.
Academic Press
,
Oxford, UK
.
Curtis
V.
,
Cousens
S.
,
Mertens
T.
,
Traore
E.
,
Kanki
B.
&
Diallo
I.
1993
Structured observations of hygiene behaviours in Burkina Faso: validity, variability, and utility
.
Bull. World Health Organ.
71
(
1
),
23
32
.
Curtis
V.
,
Schmidt
W.
,
Luby
S.
,
Florez
R.
,
Touré
O.
&
Biran
A.
2011
Hygiene: new hopes, new horizons
.
Lancet Infect. Dis.
11
,
312
321
.
Ejemot
R. I.
,
Ehiri
J. E.
,
Meremikwu
M. M.
&
Critchley
J. A.
2008
Hand washing for preventing diarrhoea
.
Cochrane Database Syst. Rev.
1
,
CD004265
.
Freeman
M. C.
,
Stocks
M. E.
,
Cumming
O.
,
Jeandron
A.
,
Higgins
J. P. T.
,
Wolf
J.
,
Prüss-Ustün
A.
,
Bonjour
S.
,
Hunter
P. R.
,
Fewtrell
L.
&
Curtis
V.
2014
Systematic review: hygiene and health: systematic review of handwashing practices worldwide and update of health effects
.
Trop. Med. Int. Health
19
(
8
),
906
916
.
IBM, Corp
2019
IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0
.
IBM Corp
,
Armonk, NY
.
Islam
M. M.
,
Jarna
R. N.
,
Khan
D. H.
,
Nayeem
M.
,
Rushmi
Z. T.
,
Rahman
M. M.
,
Paul
S.
&
Reza
H. M.
2019
Prevalence of diseases among Rohingya refugees in Bangladesh: a comprehensive study
.
IOSR J. Nurs. Health Sci.
8
,
14
21
.
Judah
G.
,
Aunger
R.
,
Schmidt
W. P.
,
Michie
S.
,
Granger
S.
&
Curtis
V.
2009
Experimental pretesting of hand-washing interventions in a natural setting
.
Am. J. Public Health
99
(
Suppl 2
),
S405
S411
.
Lopez-Quintero
C.
,
Freeman
P.
&
Neumark
Y.
2009
Hand washing among school children in Bogotá, Colombia
.
Am. J. Public Health
99
(
1
),
94
101
.
Manun'Ebo
M.
,
Cousens
S.
,
Haggerty
P.
,
Kalengaie
M.
,
Ashworth
A.
&
Kirkwood
B.
1997
Measuring hygiene practices: a comparison of questionnaires with direct observations in rural Zaire
.
Trop. Med. Int. Health
2
(
11
),
1015
1021
.
Mattioli
M. C.
,
Pickering
A. J.
,
Gilsdorf
R. J.
,
Davis
J.
&
Boehm
A. B.
2013
Hands and water as vectors of diarrheal pathogens in Bagamoyo, Tanzania
.
Environ. Sci. Technol.
47
,
355
363
.
Mosler
H. J.
&
Contzen
N.
2016
Systematic Behaviour Change in Water, Sanitation and Hygiene. A Practical Guide Using the RANAS Approach
.
Eawag
,
Dübendorf
,
Switzerland
.
O'Reilly
C. E.
,
Freeman
M. C.
,
Ravani
M.
,
Migele
J.
,
Mwaki
A.
,
Ayalo
M.
,
Ombeki
S.
,
Hoekstra
R. M.
&
Quick
R.
2008
The impact of a school-based safe water and hygiene programme on knowledge and practices of students and their parents: Nyanza Province, western Kenya, 2006
.
Epidemiol. Infect.
136
(
1
),
80
91
.
Patel
M. K.
,
Harris
J. R.
,
Juliao
P.
,
Nygren
B.
,
Were
V.
,
Kola
S.
,
Sadumah
I.
,
Faith
S. H.
,
Otieno
R.
,
Obure
A.
,
Hoekstra
R. M.
&
Quick
R.
2012
Impact of a hygiene curriculum and the installation of simple handwashing and drinking water stations in rural Kenyan primary schools on student health and hygiene practices
.
Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg.
87
(
4
),
594
601
.
Porzig-Drummond
R.
,
Stevenson
R.
,
Case
T.
&
Oaten
M.
2009
Can the emotion of disgust be harnessed to promote hand hygiene? experimental and field-based tests
.
Soc. Sci. Med.
68
(
6
),
1006
1012
.
Ram
P. K.
2013
Practical Guidance for Measuring Handwashing Behaviour: 2013 Update. Global Scaling Up Handwashing
.
Water and Sanitation Program, World Bank
,
Washington, DC
.
Ramos
M.
,
Benelli
P.
,
Irvine
E.
&
Watson
J.
2016
Handwashing: WASH in Emergencies. HIF Problem Exploration Report
.
ELRHA
,
Cardiff
.
REACH
2019
Water, Sanitation, and Hygiene Household Dry Season Follow-up Assessment
.
Cox's Bazar
,
Bangladesh
. .
Saboori
S.
,
Greene
L. E.
,
Moe
C. L.
,
Freeman
M. C.
,
Caruso
B. A.
,
Akoko
D.
&
Rheingans
R. D.
2013
Impact of regular soap provision to primary schools on hand washing and E. coli hand contamination among pupils in Nyanza Province, Kenya: a cluster-randomized trial
.
Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg.
89
(
4
),
698
708
.
Scott
B. E.
,
Lawson
D. W.
&
Curtis
V.
2007
Hard to handle: understanding mothers’ handwashing behaviour in Ghana
.
Health Policy Plan.
22
(
4
),
216
224
.
Seimetz
E.
,
Boyayo
A. M.
&
Mosler
H. J.
2016
The influence of contextual and psychosocial factors on handwashing
.
Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg.
94
(
6
),
1407
1417
.
Talaat
M.
,
Afifi
S.
,
Dueger
E.
,
El-Ashry
N.
,
Marfin
A.
,
Kandeel
A.
,
Mohareb
E.
&
El-Sayed
N.
2011
Effects of hand hygiene campaigns on incidence of laboratory-confirmed influenza and absenteeism in schoolchildren, Cairo, Egypt
.
Emerging Infect. Dis.
17
(
4
),
619
625
.
https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1704.101353
.
Vivas
A. P.
,
Gelaye
B.
,
Aboset
N.
,
Kumie
A.
,
Berhane
Y.
&
Williams
M. A.
2010
Knowledge, attitudes and practices (KAP) of hygiene among school children in Angolela, Ethiopia
.
J. Prev. Med. Hyg.
51
(
2
),
73
79
.
Wisner
B.
&
Adams
J.
2012
Environmental Health in Emergencies and Disasters: A Practical Guide
.
World Health Organization
,
Geneva
.
World Bank
2012
Behavioural Determinants of Handwashing with Soap Among Mothers and Caretakers: Emergent Learning From Senegal and Peru. Water and Sanitation Program: Learning Note
.
World Bank
,
Washington, DC
.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Licence (CC BY 4.0), which permits copying, adaptation and redistribution, provided the original work is properly cited (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Supplementary data